PainNP

Saturday, January 12, 2008

central sensitization and chronicity: bits

risk assessment for chronification: education level, dissability
central sensitization
FMS/MFD/ANSDys/ less endogenous inhibitory pain pathways

Zung and Beck score, helplessness,catastrophizing,
HKF-R 10 - screening for predicting chronicity in acute low back pain (LBP): a prospective clinical trial. - Neubauer E - Eur J Pain - 01-AUG-2006; 10(6): 559-66 (From NIH/NLM MEDLINE)

Abstract:STUDY DESIGN: Prospective cohort study. OBJECTIVES: To develop a short instrument to reliably predict chronicity in low back pain (LBP). SUMMARY OF BACKGROUND DATA: Health care expenditures on the treatment of low back pain continue to increase. It is therefore important to prevent the development of chronicity. In Germany, there is at present no early risk assessment tool to predict the risk of developing chronic LBP for patients presenting with acute LBP. Undertaken in an orthopedic practice setting, this study examined known risk factors for chronicity. It resulted in the development of a short questionnaire that successfully predicted the course of chronicity with an accuracy of 78%. METHODS: A cohort of 192 orthopaedic outpatients was assessed for clinical, behavioral, emotional, and cognitive parameters bsed on a self-report test battery of 167 established items predictive for chronicity in LBP. Chronicity was defined as back pain persisting for longer than six months. Logistic regression analysis was performed to evaluate the predictive value of all items significantly associated with the dependent variable. RESULTS: The study found the following items to have the strongest predictive value in the development of chronicity: "How strong was your back pain during the last week when it was most tolerable?" and the question "How much residual pain would you be willing to tolerate while still considering the therapy successful?" These were followed by the variables for "Duration of existing LBP" (more than eight days), the patient's educational level (low levels are related to higher risks of chronicity) and pain being experienced elsewhere in the body. Other significant factors were five items assessing depression (Zung) and the palliative effect of therapeutic massage (where a positive correlation was found). Female patients have a higher risk for chronicity, as do patients with a high total score on the scales assessing "catastrophizing thoughts" and thoughts of "helplessness". CONCLUSION: Using the items listed above, the study was able to predict a patient's risk of developing chronic LBP with a probability of 78%. These items were assembled in a brief questionnaire and were paired with a corresponding evaluative tool. This enables practitioners to assess an individual patient's risk for chronicity by means of a simple calculator in just a few minutes. A validation study for the questionnaire is currently being prepared. MINI ABSTRACT: The objective of this study was the development of a brief questionnaire to assess the risk for chronicity for LBP.
Citation:HKF-R 10 - screening for predicting chronicity in acute low back pain (LBP): a prospective clinical trial.Neubauer E - Eur J Pain - 01-AUG-2006; 10(6): 559-66From NIH/NLM MEDLINE
NLM Citation ID:
Abstract:AIM: The objective of the study was to develop a brief questionnaire to determine the risk of chronification for patients suffering from lumbar (low) back pain who are consulting a physician for the first or second time. METHOD: At the outset, and again after six months, a questionnaire with 167 valid items for chronification was distributed to patients in orthopedic offices. After six months, patients were contacted by mail to inquire whether they were still suffering from back pain. Based on outcome (persistence of back pain/absence of back pain) and by means of logistic regression analysis, those variables were determined that could predict actual chronification. RESULTS: The following items were predictive: "How strong was your back pain during the last week when it was most tolerable?" and "How much residual pain would you be willing to tolerate while still considering the therapy successful?" (Acceptance value, beta = 0.61), patient's educational level (beta = - 0.44), massage is experienced as bringing relief (beta = 0.44), 5 items of the Zung scale for depression (beta = 0.42), items of the scale for catastrophizing thoughts (beta = 0.41) and items of the scale for feelings of helplessness (beta = - 0.39) of the Kiel pain inventory; duration of the back pain for longer than 1 week (beta = 0.38), pain in other parts of the body (beta = 0.37); and female gender (beta = 0.25) CONCLUSION: Based on these questions, it was possible to predict the chronification of back pain with a probability of 78.05 %. A corresponding questionnaire and an evaluative table were developed.
Citation:[What questions are appropriate for predicting the risk of chronic disease in patients suffering from acute low back pain?]Neubauer E - Z Orthop Ihre Grenzgeb - 01-MAY-2005; 143(3): 299-301From NIH/NLM MEDLINE
NLM Citation ID:15977118 (PubMed ID)
Full Source Title:Zeitschrift fur Orthopadie und ihre Grenzgebiete
Publication Type:Clinical Trial; English Abstract; Journal Article
Language:German
Author Affiliation:Orthopädische Universitätsklinik, Heidelberg.
Authors:Neubauer E; Pirron P; Junge A; Seemann H; Schiltenwolf M

Chemical Compound Name:Welche Fragen sind geeignet, ein Chronifizierungsrisiko von akuten Rückenschmerzen vorherzusagen? Eine prospektive klinische Studie.

Monday, June 11, 2007

promotional speaker for pain web advice

Excellent article: fair balanced unbiased clinical experience and not off label, practice based learning competancy: cme vs promotional talks (ACGME, ABMS)
Promotional Talks for the Pharmaceutical Industry
Posted 07/27/2004
Thomas A. M. Kramer, MD
Medical education in the United States has begun to embrace a new paradigm -- core competencies. The idea behind this paradigm is that it is no longer sufficient to provide education to students and physicians, but that the education system must also somehow ascertain individual competency. As a point of departure for the adoption of the competency model, the Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education (the institution responsible for residency training) and the American Board of Medical Specialties (the institution responsible for Board certification) have agreed on 6 general categories of competency. A discussion of all 6 of these is beyond the scope of this column, so I will focus on one of them: "practice-based learning." Practice-based learning is defined as the ability of physicians to remain knowledgeable and current in their field throughout their career.
Traditionally, physicians have maintained currency through some combination of the current literature and continuing medical education (CME) programs in their field. In psychopharmacology, currency is particularly crucial since we have the delightful problem of multiple new agents and new uses for old agents becoming available to us on a routine basis. How do you learn how to use new medications, or discover new uses for old ones? Often, the literature is less than helpful. Clinical trials that can be easily extrapolated into clinical practice are few and far between. Moreover, although drug representatives try to tell us how to use their products, they have problems with credibility and lack of experience -- especially as sales forces for most pharmaceutical companies have evolved from having backgrounds in pharmacy training to ones in marketing training. If you are extremely lucky, you have a colleague easily accessible to you who has experience doing what you would like to learn about. Unfortunately, rarely are we able to find such a colleague. Thus we come to the most positive and constructive reason for drug company-sponsored programs. Colleagues need to share their experience. Pharmaceutical companies have the resources to facilitate such sharing by having a practitioner who has had experience with their product describe it to other practitioners.
It is important to understand that there are 2 distinctly different kinds of pharmaceutical industry-sponsored programs. The key distinction between them is whether they are being given for formal (accredited) CME credit. Pharmaceutical funding for CME programs is unrestricted, meaning the money is paid as a grant, with no influence over the content. Promotional programs, on the other hand, are paid for by the pharmaceutical company with a clear and unambiguous goal of promoting their product.
I started doing drug talks of both varieties in the early 1990s. I enjoyed doing them immensely. I love to teach psychopharmacology, and this was an opportunity to do that and get paid extra money. Having been in academics for my whole career, these talks provided me with an opportunity to get out and talk to full-time practitioners about the realities of their practice, and I learned from them at least as much as I taught them. Moreover, I never felt any degree of compromise of my principles. The clear message I got from the drug reps who hired me was to go out there and be entertaining. If I could say something nice about their product, that was fine, but certainly I never felt it was required. As one rep said to me: "It's my job to be the salesman. Your job is to provide a reason to get all the docs in the room and create goodwill so they will listen to me after they listen to you." I worked for a number of competing pharmaceutical companies and was proud of the fact that they would all compliment me for being fair, balanced, and unbiased.
Four years ago, I took a new job that required me to have no outside income or any professional connection to anything except the institution that was my employer. As a result, I severed all ties with the pharmaceutical industry -- and was thus truly out of the loop for this period of time. About a year and a half ago, I left that job and now have a job in which nonclinical outside income is not a problem. I wanted to get back to doing drug talks.
What awaited me was a welcome to a new and different world. The process of doing CME talks (eg, Grand Rounds presentations, etc.) has not changed very much, and I have had a good time doing a few of those. Promotional talks, however, are a very different story. Recent litigation about pharmaceutical companies actively marketing off-label indications to physicians and increased interest in pharmaceutical marketing practices by the federal government have made pharmaceutical companies considerably more restrictive about what can and cannot be said at a promotional talk.
One company in particular insisted that I attend a 2-day speaker training program before I did talks for them. When I pointed out that I had done over 150 drug talks, they were unmoved. They said that the training program was a requirement for speakers, and I quickly found out why. A substantial portion of the training involved explaining to us what we could not say. Anything that was not explicitly stated in the labeling information (product insert) for the drug could not be said in the talk. They gave us the slides that we were to use. We could not add any slides, even if it was purely disease-related material or just cartoons. And we could not discuss our individual clinical experience. The only exception to the requirements came with questions: if we were asked direct questions about off-label uses or anything else that was not in the product insert (such as our clinical experience), we were allowed to answer them.
This is the new world of promotional drug talks. In contrast to my old drug rep's statement, we were told at speaker training: "When you are giving a talk for us, at that time you are an employee of this company and have to abide by all the restrictions that any employee would in dealing with physicians." In many respects, we have come full circle. Many years ago, as noted above, drug reps for the most part were pharmacists. Later they were marketers, and now, at least in part, physicians are being asked to do some of those same marketing tasks in talks not dissimilar to the drug reps' presentations.
Are there ways around or even out of this conundrum? Certainly doing these presentations in a way that encourages a lot of question-and-answer interaction will bring them closer to what they used to be -- that is, a frank discussion of what physicians in attendance are seeking: your clinical experience with the drug. Attendees may learn that they are better served by asking the speaker pointed and direct questions. Whether physicians will continue to be interested in attending programs in which the speaker gives a similar talk to what they have already heard from the drug rep in their office remains to be seen. Most importantly, we have an obligation as practitioners to educate each other. Clearly, the involvement of the pharmaceutical industry in that process is a moving target.
Thomas A. M. Kramer, MD, Associate Professor of Psychiatry, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
Disclosure: Thomas A. M. Kramer, MD, has no significant financial interests or relationships to disclose.

Labels:

Saturday, June 17, 2006

urinary retension and pain

Acute urinary retention:
stream diminishing Analgesia, duplex scan, US, MRI

Torsion of a testicular appendage results from ischemia and infarction of an embryologic remnant. Although four possible remnants are at risk, the appendix testis and appendix epididymis account for approximately 99% of all cases. Although the blue dot sign may be visible early in the process, it eventually is obscured by edema and erythema. There are rarely any voiding symptoms or fever associated with this entity. Doppler ultrasonography typically reveals normal testicular blood flow with a small hyperechoic area adjacent to the testis. Once the diagnosis is definitively made, management is nonoperative. Analgesics and scrotal support may be useful supportive adjuncts.
Epididymitis (PID!!!!) arises from pain and swelling of the epididymis. It usually arises secondary to infection or inflammation from the urethra or bladder. If the process remains untreated, it may involve the adjacent testis and scrotum, and eventually result in abscess formation. Fever and leukocytosis are present in between 30% and 50% of cases. Doppler ultrasonography reveals normal to increased blood flow to the testis and epididymis, contrasting sharply with testis torsion. Antibiotic treatment for epididymitis depends on patient age and probable underlying pathogen. Neisseria gonorrhoeae and Chlamydia trachomatis account for most cases in men under 35, and these may be treated with intramuscular ceftriaxone plus a course of doxycycline. In men over 35, urine culture usually reveals Escherichia coli, and treatment consists of an oral fluoroquinolone for 21 days.
The most common infectious cause for acute retention is acute prostatitis. Patients typically appear sick at presentation, displaying fever, dysuria, and perineal pain. Digital rectal examination reveals an exquisitely tender, boggy prostate. Aggressive prostate may cause bacteremia, and should be avoided. Patients should begin therapy immediately, and toxic patients may need to be admitted for monitoring, fluid resuscitation, and intravenous antibiotics. If the patient is in urinary retention, a small urethral catheter should be gently inserted. Alternatively, a suprapubic catheter may be placed if patients find a urethral Foley catheter too uncomfortable. Other infectious causes of retention include urethral herpes, periurethral abscesses, and tuberculous cystitis
Neurovascular Hx: Sickle cell disease and other thromboembolic or hypercoagulable states (eg, polycythemia, thalassemia, and vasculitis) are frequently associated with priapism
obstructive, infectious, pharmacologic, and neurogenic: Neurogenic/pathic
There are many pharmacologic agents that may contribute to urinary retention. The most common group includes anticholinergics. These may act by blocking postganglionic impulses to the detrusor and inhibiting contraction. Common offending medications include atropine, oxybutynin, tolterodine, and certain antihistamines. α-Adrenergic agonists (commonly found in decongestants) including ephedrine and pseudoephedrine cause bladder neck smooth muscle contraction and subsequent retention. Psychiatric medications including phenothiazine antipsychotics and monoamine oxidase inhibitors also have anticholinergic effects and may cause acute retention. Withdrawal of the offending medication may ultimately relieve retention.
hyperplasia, tumor: painless gross hematuria
prastatic:12F/ coude decompression: palpate >1000cc in,
strictures, spasms, stones, FB,
urodynamic studies,
phimosis: unretracted/ paraphimosis: triamcinolone:
paraphimosis: block and conscious sedation and ace glans to return through the phimotic ring. Or dorsal slit and circ elective
Advanced prostate cancer, which invades the urethra or bladder neck, may also cause acute urinary retention. On rectal examination, these patients typically have nodular, indurated prostates with palpable tumor, which may extend beyond the border of the gland. They typically have elevated serum prostate-specific antigen levels, and may have undergone transrectal ultrasound-guided prostate biopsy for tissue diagnosis. They may have signs of systemic disease, including bone pain and weight loss. Treatment is by transurethral catheter placement, and urologic consultation regarding future prostate cancer therapeutic options.
Urethral strictures not infrequently present in acute retention. The stricture is a circumferential scar of the mucosa and underlying corpus spongiosum. The stricture may be anywhere from the urethral meatus back to the bladder neck. Strictures usually arise secondary to previous urethral instrumentation, perineal trauma, or urethritis, but the etiology is frequently idiopathic. The stricture may occasionally be palpable through the scrotal or perineal skin as a firm, indurated region of urethra. A small Foley catheter (12F catheter) may successfully traverse the narrowed segment. If not, the stricture may be dilated using urethral filiforms and followers up to 12F catheter for catheter placement. Alternatively, a suprapubic catheter may be placed for bladder decompression. Both of these procedures should be performed only by those experienced in these maneuvers, and urologic consultation is often prudent to avoid significant urethral trauma.
Bladder stones or bladder tumors may also cause obstructive urinary retention. A bladder stone typically forms secondary to urinary stasis (because of bladder outlet obstruction) or a foreign body in the bladder. These patients typically complain of intermittency because the stone acts as a ball valve at the bladder neck. A urethral catheter may temporarily push the stone back up into the bladder and relieve the obstruction. The bladder stone may ultimately be treated by transurethral fragmentation or open cystolithotomy for larger calculi. If the cause of the stone is an obstructing prostate adenoma, it is typically resected or excised simultaneously.
A bladder tumor may lead to urinary retention through significant hematuria and blood clot formation. A patient who presents with painless gross hematuria and clots should be suspected of harboring an underlying bladder tumor. As the clots solidify and increase in number, they may settle at the most dependent portion of the bladder (the bladder neck) and completely block outflow. Most patients have some interval of gross hematuria and pass a few clots before clot retention occurs. The best treatment for these patients is placement of a large three-way hematuria catheter for continuous bladder irrigation. Aggressive manual clot irrigation is critical to ensure adequate drainage. Patients may occasionally require urgent operative intervention for clot evacuation and fulguration of hemorrhage in anticipation of formal transurethral bladder tumor resection. Patients may bleed significantly before presentation, and routine hematocrit checks are prudent in this situation.
Less common obstructive etiologies include urethral foreign bodies, penile constricting bands, and meatal stenosis. Obstructive urinary retention in women is distinctly uncommon, but causes may include ureteroceles, urethral polyps, and urethral strictures.
Neurogenic causes of urinary retention may be broadly categorized into upper motor neuron lesions, lower motor neuron lesions, and peripheral nerve lesions. Upper motor neuron lesions include all neurologic lesions above the sacral micturition center. The most common cerebral upper motor neuron lesions include cerebrovascular accident (stroke); multiple sclerosis; and Parkinson's disease. In the suprasacral cord, tumors and spinal cord injury are frequent causes. The bladder is typically spastic, with decreased storage capacity and impaired emptying secondary to sphincter dyssynergia. Lower motor neuron lesions include spinal cord trauma below S1, spinal cord compression secondary to metastatic cancer, and multiple sclerosis. The bladder is usually hypotonic or atonic in these cases. Peripheral nerve lesions that commonly lead to urinary retention include diabetic neuropathy; intervertebral disk herniation (specifically L4-5 or L5-S1); and pelvic surgery (abdominoperineal resection for colon cancer, radical hysterectomy), which may inadvertently injure the pelvic plexus.
Treatment options for patients with neurogenic urinary retention should be individualized according to the patient's overall clinical and neurologic status. These patients frequently have complex voiding dysfunction, and urodynamic studies are often of benefit, particularly in patients with spinal cord injury. Treatment options may range from clean intermittent catheterization to bladder augmentation and catheterizable stoma creation. Suprapubic or indwelling urethral catheter placement is much less desirable in the long-term and should be reserved only for patients with a poor prognosis or those who are unable or unwilling to learn intermittent catheterization.
SummaryGenitourinary emergencies are commonly seen in the emergency room, and the primary care physician plays a vital role in the initial evaluation and treatment of each. Although genitourinary trauma is rarely life threatening, it may be the cause of significant long-term morbidity. Key clinical indicators outlined in this article (eg, inability to urinate, gross hematuria) combined with judicious use of imaging help stage the injury and allow a safe and rational approach to treatment. The acute scrotum frequently presents a challenging problem to both the emergentologist and urologist. Although epididymitis may be managed nonoperatively, there should be no delay in exploring suspected testis torsion. The conditions of the penis outlined require urgent treatment to preserve potency (priapism) and restore normal function (eg, penile amputation). Acute urinary retention has a myriad of underlying etiologies, and treatment must be individualized. Urgent bladder decompression by urethral or suprapubic catheterization provides initial relief until urologic consultation is available.

Wednesday, January 11, 2006

Psych History is midbrain only to Pain

not an original work

. Which principles guide the organization and presentation of clinical data?
After you have done the initial interview(s), performed the mental status exam, and gathered the results of various tests, you are left with the task of organizing and presenting the data coherently. This task often is daunting. Keep in mind that your primary goal is to be able to tell a concise yet sufficiently detailed story about the patient’s current state so that (1) you have at least a few working hypotheses about the patient’s problems, and (2) a person hearing (or reading) about the patient has enough information to arrive at his or her own hypotheses. The success of any effort toward organization and presentation of information is founded on the clearest presentation of the most relevant facts.

2. Where do I start?
The psychiatric presentation differs little from the standard way of presenting a medical patient, and often is organized in the following order:


a. Chief complaint f. Family psychiatric history
b. History of present illness g. Physical exam
c. Past psychiatric history h. Mental status exam
d. Past medical history i. Assessment and plan
e. Psychosocial history



3. How do the write-ups for a patient with schizophrenia and a patient with diabetes differ?
In theory, nothing is different; in practice, however, psychiatrists tend to work more effectively when they have even a rudimentary grasp of who the patient is as a person, and not just as the vehicle for an array of signs and symptoms. Of course, this could be said to be true for all physicians, not just psychiatrists. However, because psychiatrists deal with disturbances in patients’ behaviors, thoughts, moods, and feelings, a vivid and lifelike description of the patient’s history can be especially helpful in diagnosis and treatment.

4. What does assessing the patient “as a person” mean in practice?
Compare these two hypothetical histories of present illness:

Mr. Jones is a 54-year-old married white man who was in his usual state of health until 3 weeks prior to admission, when he lost his job. He then noted subacute onset of early-morning awakening, weight loss, fatigue, decreased concentration, and depressed mood. His wife, noticing that he had suicidal ideation, brought him to the hospital.

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

13
Mr. Jones is a 54-year-old married white man who derived much of his sense of self-esteem and accomplishment from his job as an accountant, which he held for over 30 years. Three weeks ago, when he was laid off (due to cutbacks in the firm where he worked), he felt that “The rug was pulled out from under me.” He had always made his job the center of his life, and was left with nothing to do at home. His wife was overwhelmed as well. He says that he began to feel that “Nothing made sense any more … I felt all washed up,” and gradually lost his appetite as well as his interest in and energy for the few hobbies he had. He developed insomnia with early-morning awakening, and after a while began to tell his wife that “I’m no use to anyone anymore.” When he asked his wife if she’d miss him “if he were gone,” she brought him to the hospital.

Although both histories give a clear picture of someone developing an episode of major depression, the second one conveys a more detailed and useful assessment of the patient as a person, as well as of the circumstances leading up to the hospital admission. It presents at least the beginning of an understanding of the patient’s personality and home life. The use of direct quotations greatly contributes to the sense of vividness and immediacy, and begins to help the listener empathize with the patient’s suffering. When reading or hearing such a presentation, important questions readily come to mind: Had Mr. Jones seen the cutbacks coming but failed to plan for them? Why was the job the center of his life? Why couldn’t his wife have been more of a support? Why does he frame his anguish in terms of his not being any “use” to anyone?—and so forth.

The essential point is to make the history of present illness as vivid and richly detailed as possible, making use of direct patient quotations when applicable and useful, and minimizing use of standard “boiler-plate” jargon about symptoms and behavior.

5. What sort of boiler-plate jargon should be avoided?
The kind that describes symptoms in concise but impoverished ways that lack important complexity. For example, rather than saying:

“The patient demonstrated intense affect when faced with his illness.”

Consider saying instead:

“The patient became tearful and sad when discussing the isolation caused by his psychotic thinking.”

Rather than saying:

“The patient exhibited agitated and threatening behavior in her relationship.”

Consider saying:

“The patient shouted angrily and shook her fist at her boyfriend.”

Once again, the more successful you are at depicting a clear, detailed, and evocative history, the greater the chances will be of reaching a more accurate assessment that leads directly to a rational treatment plan. Avoid clinical clichés!

6. How should the past psychiatric history be incorporated?
This part of the presentation should document not only what prior treatment the patient has received, but also the circumstances and outcome of such treatment. Outlining untreated episodes of illness also can be helpful, as can describing the initial onset of symptoms.

Hospitalizations:
Note precipitating factors, length of stay, success/failure of different treatments used, working diagnoses.

Somatic treatments:
Note dosage of medication, duration, usefulness, and side effects. If appropriate, mention whether electroconvulsive therapy has been used.

Therapy:
Note session length, frequency, duration, focus (e.g., supportive, exploratory, behavioral, cognitive), and usefulness.

Suicidality/homicidality:
Note stressors, prior attempts (in detail), and treatment measures that proved effective. Classifying prior attempts in terms of relative risk and rescue potential can be particularly helpful. For example: the patient who lightly lacerated a wrist in full view of a family member would be considered a low-risk/high-rescue attempt; taking an overdose of acetaminophen behind a locked bathroom door would be high-risk/low-rescue.


--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

14
Remember: eliminate vagueness! Saying “The patient failed a lithium trial” is less helpful than saying “A two-month trial of lithium at therapeutic plasma levels resulted in intolerable tremor and polyuria.” Direct patient quotations can be quite useful, as well.

7. Is taking a past medical history in a psychiatric setting different than in a nonpsychiatric setting?
Not substantially. Clearly, any illness with possible psychiatric complications (e.g., Parkinson’s disease, multiple sclerosis, stroke, Lyme disease, hyopthyroidism) should be explored in some detail. Given the protean psychiatric manifestations of several different types of seizures, the presence or absence of a seizure disorder is especially important to determine. Mood disorders, hallucinations, delusions, and unusual character traits may be sequelae of a seizure focus, usually in the temporolimbic area. Because these symptoms may represent ictal manifestations of a nonconvulsive seizure, a patient’s seizures may have gone undiagnosed. Therefore, inquire about any history of head trauma, particularly if it led to loss of consciousness.

8. Describe some key points of the psychosocial history.
Given the importance of early relationships to personality development and coping skills, a few essential facts about the patient’s upbringing can shed light on his or her current functioning. A brief outline of the structure of the patient’s family is essential, and should include the place of the patient in the birth order, whether or not the parents were ever married or remain married, and whether either parent is deceased. Patients often fail to spontaneously mention losses of other family members; therefore, ask whether any siblings or grandparents were lost to the patient either recently or in childhood.

Physical and sexual abuse—two obviously sensitive subjects—should be mentioned tactfully but candidly if they are clinically relevant. Clearly, taking a history of abuse must be done with care, with special attention to the patient’s clinical condition and whether or not discussion of such events could be traumatizing or intrusive.

Information about the patient’s work history and relationships is invaluable for an accurate assessment of personality functioning. How does the patient respond to the responsibility of a job? How does he or she deal with interpersonal conflict and intimacy? Similarly, even a brief history of how the patient performed in school can convey a sense of early social relationships as well as any possible learning difficulties that may have gone undetected.

Substance abuse often is presented in this section. The same overall suggestions for detail and richness apply, e.g.: “The patient typically drinks alone, on weekends only, consuming ‘anything I can get my hands on’ until he passes out. He has never had seizures or DTs, and does not like Alcoholics Anonymous because ‘it’s hard to be around so many strangers.’” This description is so much more evocative than “Patient is alcohol-dependent.”

The patient’s religion and whether it is important also should be mentioned, especially if the patient is struggling with suicidal impulses.

Finally, any military and legal elements are important and should not be neglected.

9. What about the family psychiatric history?
A careful exploration of the family’s history of mental illness frequently offers helpful data. Not uncommonly, patients remember that a family member was psychiatrically ill but don’t know the diagnosis. Certain facts can be revealing: The patient’s mother was “nervous a lot” and was hospitalized five times for “shock treatments.” The patient’s uncle was “always hyper,” “drank too much,” and was arrested three times for passing bad checks.

Not surprisingly, such details often are more revealing than the “diagnosis” remembered by the patient.

10. What are the pitfalls in presenting the mental status exam?
This is where boiler-plate jargon can easily get out of hand (see Question 5). “Patient has auditory hallucinations” can be true, if the examiner was careful, or quite false, if the exam was cursory. Be specific! If a patient mentions hearing voices, you need to ask: How many voices? Are they there


--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

15
all the time? Do they comment on the patient? Are they perceived as coming from inside or outside the head? Do they tell the patient to do anything? Are they threatening or comforting? Don’t just mention the symptom—describe it. Describe the patient’s appearance, interpersonal style, and mannerisms or idiosyncrasies. This principle applies to all portions of the mental status exam, including cognitive testing.

11. How is everything pulled together in the formulation (or assessment) section?
Remarkably little consensus exists about what it means to formulate a case. A common belief is that formulation involves an esoteric and sophisticated explanation of the patient’s difficulties that displays the examiner’s ability to extrapolate more from details of the case than meets the eye. Rather, perhaps the essence of formulation is that it arranges facts in such a way as to suggest a differential diagnosis and a treatment plan. A focus on basic psychiatric knowledge, common sense, and a willingness to think in terms of hypotheses rather than conclusions usually lead to a clarifying and useful formulation.

12. Give some examples of successful formulations.
One main function of the formulation is to summarize known pertinent clinical facts, emphasizing the stressors and sequence of events that led to the patient seeking help. Features essential to any formulation are:

• An indication of baseline functioning (no prior psychosis)
• A description of a likely stressor (the loss of the job)
• A response to that stressor (humiliation, followed by cocaine use)
• A summary of the salient symptomatic phenomenology (e.g., grandiosity, irritability)
• A differential diagnosis

The following is a simple but useful formulation:

In summary, Ms. Smith is a 19-year-old woman with the new onset of a psychotic disorder that has developed over the past 3 weeks. The symptoms appeared to begin fairly abruptly after she was fired from her job and began using cocaine daily to avoid her feelings of shame and disappointment. Her grandiosity, irritability, sleeplessness, and pressured speech all suggest the diagnosis of bipolar disorder, manic phase; however, given the amount of cocaine that she has been using, it is worth considering the possibility of a substance-induced mood disorder with manic features, secondary to cocaine.

The hypothesis that the patient used cocaine to avoid the pain of her loss is clearly based more on common sense and empathy rather than on any formula concerning human behavior. Here is a more complex formulation (referring to the patient in Question 4):

In summary, Mr. Jones is a man without prior psychiatric history whose self-esteem is closely tied to his ability to be an effective worker and to provide for his family. The loss of his job is a tremendous blow to his self-image and quickly has led to his feeling worthless, guilty, and hopeless. He has developed all the signs of a major depressive episode. His suicidal state may be the outcome of his hopelessness and his inward-turned rage about the layoff.

13. The previous formulation indicates that the patient is turning his rage onto himself. Explain.
That is a hypothesis based on psychodynamic theory, which holds that human behavior is, to a large extent, governed by hidden (or, more specifically, unconscious) meanings and forces within the mind. Psychodynamic principles can be useful tools for probing and unraveling patients’ difficulties; these principles are most helpful when used to generate hypotheses, which then require further data to be confirmed or refuted. In this case, for example, further discussions with this patient while he was recovering clinically might reveal the rage (perhaps directed against a harsh and over-demanding father) masked by the acute symptoms. Exploration and venting of the rage might lead to further improvement and reduced vulnerability to future depression.

14. Are there other hypotheses that can be used as tools in case formulation?
Three other sets of hypotheses, which are well-summarized by Lazare, are the sociocultural, the behavioral, and the biologic/syndromal. Although a complete description of each of these is


--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

16
beyond the scope of this chapter, some familiarity with each can greatly enhance a clinician’s skill in reaching an accurate and thorough formulation. The biologic/syndromal approach underlies the classification system contained in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-IV) of the American Psychiatric Association, which is the prevailing diagnostic system within American psychiatry.

15. What if the formulation is wrong?
Revise it. The value of a formulation is that it provides a starting point for an informed understanding and discussion of the case. It is less important to be “right” than to be flexible. Think of the initial formulation as leading to a working diagnosis that will guide the initial work-up and treatment and may be modified as you become more familiar with the patient. In summary:

• Emphasize clinical detail.
• Avoid clinical clichés and boiler-plate jargon.
• Quote the patient where applicable.
• Describe symptoms rather than label them.
• Use the formulation to summarize the facts, generate hypotheses, and arrive at a working

Monday, September 19, 2005

AACPI's key messages 2005

Key Messages
___________________________________________

Pain is a national public health crisis. It is Our Nation's Hidden Epidemic.
Ø More than 75 million Americans suffer serious pain each year.
Ø Pain is the number one reason people seek medical care.
Ø Uncontrolled pain diminishes quality of life and decreases work productivity.
Ø Pain has serious economic consequences—pain costs our economy $100 billion in medical costs and lost workdays.
Under treatment of pain has serious physiological, psychological, and social consequences.

Ø Pain weakens the immune system and slows recovery from disease or injury.
Ø Uncontrolled pain diminishes the quality of life, adversely affecting almost every aspect of a person’s life including sleep, work, social and sexual relations.
Ø Pain causes anxiety and depression, and may lead to thoughts of suicide.

Most pain can be managed or greatly eased with proper pain management.

Ø When pain is treated properly, many people can resume their normal lives, and many can experience a better quality of life.

Unfortunately, there are barriers that prevent effective pain treatment
Ø Most healthcare professionals have little or no training in pain management (and are unable to effectively respond to patients' reports of pain).
Ø Exaggerated concerns about addiction lead to under treatment of pain. Addiction to opioid analgesics is unlikely for persons with no history of substance abuse and when opioids are properly prescribed and taken under medical supervision.
Ø Pain carries a stigma. Many people with pain are fearful or embarrassed to let their families, friends, and even their healthcare professionals know they are in pain because they don't want to appear weak, or be considered a bad patient.
Ø Some government policies impede pain relief by restricting access to pain treatment

journal search on self report of pain

pain journals 10/04
PainNP
View
Delete

Self-reported management of pain in hospitalized patients: link between process and outcome.Bovier PA - Am J Med - 15-OCT-2004; 117(8): 569-74From NIH/NLM MEDLINE NLM Citation ID:15465505 (PubMed)Full Source Title:American Journal of MedicinePublication Type:Journal ArticleLanguage:EnglishAuthor Affiliation:Quality of Care Unit, Geneva University Hospitals, Switzerland. patrick.bovier@hcuge.chAuthors:Bovier PA; Charvet A; Cleopas A; Vogt N; Perneger TVAbstract:PURPOSE: Hospitalized patients commonly experience pain. We investigated the association between patients' reported use of recommended pain management practices and overall pain relief. METHODS: All adult patients discharged during a 1-month period from a Swiss teaching hospital were invited to complete a mailed survey that included the Picker patient experience questionnaire, questions on pain relief during hospitalization, and questions on various procedures that are recommended as standards of pain management. RESULTS: Of 2156 eligible patients, 1518 (70%) participated. Sixty-nine percent (n = 1050) had experienced pain during their hospital stay, of whom 71% (n = 697/978) reported complete pain relief. After adjustment for sex, age, general health, and hospital department, pain relief was associated independently with availability of physicians (odds ratio [OR] = 11; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 3.3 to 36 for excellent vs. poor availability), having received information about pain and its management (OR = 2.8; 95% CI: 1.8 to 4.2), regular pain assessment (OR = 1.8; 95% CI: 1.2 to 2.8), modification of pain treatment when ineffective (OR = 3.0; 95% CI: 1.6 to 5.6), and waiting less than 10 minutes for pain medications (OR = 3.5; 95% CI: 1.9 to 6.6). CONCLUSION: Patient reports that recommended pain management procedures had been used were associated with better self-reported pain relief among hospitalized patienPATIENT-REPORTED USE OF RECOMMENDED PRACTICES Reported use of recommended pain management practices varied substantially (Table 1). Of the 564 patients who asked for pain medication, 20% received it immediately, 41% waited 1 to 5 minutes, 20% waited 6 to 10 minutes, and 19% waited 11 minutes or more. Forty-four percent of patients rated the availability of doctors as very good to excellent, and 49% thought the same of nurses. The majority of patients (53%) received enough information about pain and its management, 60% reported regular pain assessment, and 53% were always relieved. When pain was not relieved by treatment, 72% of patients had their treatment changed. Systematic use of a pain assessment tool was reported by only 33% of patients.ASSOCIATIONS WITH PAIN RELIEF Reported use of most pain management recommendations, except for access to a patient-controlled analgesic device, was associated with self-reported complete pain relief (Table 1). Shorter waiting time and better availability of doctors and nurses were also associated with a higher likelihood of complete pain relief.After adjustment for sex, age, general health, and hospital department, five medical care factors remained associated with self-reported complete pain relief, including availability of doctors, information about pain and its management, regular pain assessment, modification of treatment, and short average waiting time before receiving a pain killer (Table 3). When none of the five factors was reported, only 11% of patients noted pain relief, but when all were reported, 95% of patients did (Figure).Table 3 . Medical Care Factors Associated with Self-Reported Complete Pain Relief in a Multivariate Logistic Regression Model Characteristic Odds Ratio* (95% Confidence Interval) Availability of medical doctor Poor 1.0 – Fair 2.3 (0.8–7.0) Good 4.0 (1.4–11) Very good 5.9 (2.0–17) Excellent 11 (3.3–36) Received information about pain and its management (definitely vs. to some extent or did not receive) 2.8 (1.8–4.2) Regular assessment of pain (vs. irregular or none) 1.8 (1.2–2.8) Treatment modification (vs. no modification) Pain was always relieved 11 (5.8–20) Treatment was modified 3.0 (1.6–5.6) Average waiting time before receiving a pain medication Never asked 3.7 (1.9–7.2) 0–10 minutes 3.5 (1.9–6.6) >10 minutes 1.0 – * Adjusted for age, sex, general health, and type of medical service. Figure Relation between the number of recommended pain management processes reported by patients and self-reported pain relief among hospitalized patients. Factors included very good or excellent availability of medical doctor; definitely received information about pain and its management; regular assessment of pain; modification of treatment or pain always relieved; never asked for a pain medication; and waiting time of 10 minutes or less. DISCUSSIONWe found that reported use of recommended pain management procedures during routine care was associated with better self-reported pain relief among hospitalized patients. A majority of patients (69%) said they had experienced pain, many of whom had inadequate pain relief. Pain relief was more common in patients who reported they had received information about pain and its management, regular pain assessment, modification of pain treatment when necessary, and short average waiting time before receiving a requested pain medication. These results confirm that several of the pain management processes recommended by international guidelines are associated with better self-reported pain relief among patients. However, one process indicator—use of a patient-controlled analgesic device (23)—was not associated with pain relief. We believe that process indicators associated with good outcomes are most useful for monitoring quality of care.We also found that women, patients who reported being in poor health, and patients who felt depressed at the time of the survey were more likely to report unsatisfactory pain relief. We cannot determine whether these characteristics identify patients who are less tolerant of pain or who are less likely to receive effective analgesia in the hospital.From the patient's standpoint, the availability of doctors and nurses appears to affect pain management. In our study, perceived availability of doctors was associated significantly with pain relief, whereas the availability of nurses was related to pain relief in univariate analysis but not after adjustment for specific medical care variables. A likely explanation is that medical care is mostly carried out by nurses, who perform regular pain assessments and respond to patients who request a pain medication. Thus, nurse availability matters because it is a requirement for successful implementation of pain management guidelines.Previous studies suggest that a good relationship between patient and physician is linked with successful pain management (24, 25). For a patient, being given undivided attention and being shown empathy by one's doctor can have an analgesic effect (24). Our results suggest that the availability of doctors may influence pain relief through mechanisms that were not captured by the questionnaire, such as the prescription of effective doses of analgesics.We relied exclusively on patient reports of pain relief and pain-related processes using a survey that was administered several weeks after hospitalization. This raises the issue of recall bias. Nevertheless, retrospective pain assessments appear to be valid for a 3-month period (26). Because we relied on patient reports, we were not able to measure some important pain management processes, such as type of analgesia or dosage used. Furthermore, our questions may not have allowed patients to discriminate between pain and other types of suffering, such as emotional distress, which are unlikely to be alleviated by analgesics. Finally, as with any cross-sectional study, causal interpretation of our findings must be done cautiously.However, we studied a large sample of patients from several services at a general hospital, and the participation rate was reasonable. Finally, our results were based on the views of patients, whose judgment is essential when dealing with pain management (9).In conclusion, we found that at a large general teaching hospital, the majority of patients experienced pain during hospitalization. Pain relief was inadequate in a substantial minority. Recommended pain management practices were applied inconsistently, even though many were associated with better pain relief. Hospitals should be encouraged to apply these practices more consistently.Acknowledgment The authors wish to thank the Quality of Care Program of Geneva University HospitalsPAIN AND REGIONAL ANESTHESIA--------------------------------------------------------------------------------Experimental Pain Models Reveal No Sex Differences in Pentazocine Analgesia in HumansRoger B. Fillingim, Ph.D.,*Timothy J. Ness, M.D., Ph.D.,†Toni L. Glover, M.A., R.N.,‡Claudia M. Campbell, B.A.,§Donald D. Price, Ph.D.,‖Roland Staud, M.D.#* Associate Professor, University of Florida College of Dentistry, Public Health Services and Research, and Gainesville Veterans Affairs Medical Center. † Professor, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Department of Anesthesiology, Birmingham, Alabama. ‡ Research Nurse, University of Florida College of Dentistry, Public Health Services and Research. § Graduate Assistant, University of Florida College of Dentistry, Public Health Services and Research, and University of Florida, Department of Clinical and Health Psychology. ‖ Professor, University of Florida College of Dentistry, Department of Oral Surgery. # Associate Professor, University of Florida, Department of Medicine.Received from the University of Florida and the Gainesville Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Gainesville, Florida.Submitted for publication October 17, 2003.Accepted for publication January 22, 2004.--------------------------------------------------------------------------------This material is the result of work supported with resources and the use of facilities at the Malcom Randall Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Gainesville, Florida. Supported by grant No. NS41670 and General Clinical Research Center Grant No. RR00082 from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland.Address reprint requests to Dr. Fillingim: University of Florida College of Dentistry, Public Health Services and Research, 1600 Southwest Archer Road, Room D8-44A, P. O. Box 100404, Gainesville, Florida 32610-0404. Address electronic mail to: rfilling@ufl.edu. Individual article reprints may be purchased through the Journal Web site, www.anesthesiology.org.Background: Accumulating evidence suggests that there are sex differences in analgesic responses to opioid agonists. Several studies using an oral surgery pain model have reported more robust analgesia to κ-agonist–antagonists (e.g., pentazocine, nalbuphine, butorphanol) among women than among men. However, evidence of sex differences in κ-agonist–antagonist effects from studies of experimentally induced pain in humans is lacking.Methods: Therefore, the analgesic effects of intravenous pentazocine (0.5 mg/kg) were determined in healthy women (n = 41) and men (n = 38) using three experimental pain models: heat pain, pressure pain, and ischemic pain. Each pain procedure was conducted before and after double-blind administration of both pentazocine and saline, which occurred on separate days in counterbalanced order.Results: Compared with saline, pentazocine produced significant analgesic responses for all pain stimuli. However, no sex differences in pentazocine analgesia emerged. Effect sizes for the sex differences were computed; the magnitude of effects was small, and an equal number of measures showed greater analgesia in men than in women. Also, analgesic responses were not highly correlated across pain modalities, suggesting that different mechanisms may underlie analgesia for disparate types of pain.Conclusions: These findings indicate significant analgesic responses to pentazocine in both men and women across multiple experimental pain assays, and the absence of sex differences contrasts with previous data from the oral surgery model. The most likely explanation for the discrepancy in results is that of differences in the pain assays. These findings are important because they suggest that sex differences in opioid analgesia may be specific to certain types of pain.IntroductionOPIOID analgesic responses are characterized by substantial individual differences, and an understanding of the factors contributing to this variability is of tremendous clinical and scientific importance. In this regard, sex-related influences on responses to opioids have received increasing attention in recent years. A recent review indicated that women consume significantly less opioid medication postoperatively than men do. [1] However, because many of these studies failed to assess pain, it is difficult to determine whether the lower opioid consumption in women was due to enhanced analgesia or other factors (e.g., side effects). A more recent investigation of nearly 2,300 patients found that female patients had similar or lower postsurgical pain ratings than male patients even though they consumed 23.5, 37.5, and 43% less opioid than the male patients on postoperative days 1, 2, and 3, respectively. [2] In addition to these findings from postoperative studies, which almost exclusively involve μ opioids, sex differences in μ-opioid analgesia have been demonstrated using experimental pain models. Sarton et al. [3] examined morphine analgesia among 10 healthy women and 10 healthy men using an electrical pain model. Women showed greater analgesic potency but slower onset and offset of analgesia. These authors had previously reported greater morphine-induced respiratory depression among women than among men. [4] [5] Zacny [6] used two experimental pain models (pressure and cold pressor pain) to determine sex differences in analgesic responses to three μ-opioid agonists, morphine, meperidine, and hydromorphone, in a sample of 16 male and 15 female patients. No sex differences in analgesia emerged for pressure pain; however, analgesic responses for all three drugs were greater among female patients for cold pressor pain. Therefore, evidence from both laboratory and clinical studies suggests that women may experience greater μ-opioid analgesia than men.A series of studies that have garnered considerable scientific and media attention has investigated sex differences in analgesic responses to κ-agonist–antagonist medications using an oral surgery model. These investigators first reported greater analgesic responses among female patients compared with male patients for pentazocine but not morphine. [7] [8] Subsequently, they demonstrated more prolonged analgesia among female patients than among male patients with the κ-agonist–antagonists nalbuphine and butorphanol. [9] More recently, they have demonstrated that after low-dose nalbuphine (5 mg), pain ratings increased in men but showed no change in women, whereas higher doses (10 and 20 mg) produced analgesia of longer duration in women than in men. [10] These results indicate more robust analgesic responses to κ-agonist–antagonist medications among women but no differences in morphine analgesia. We recently demonstrated that the melanocortin-1 receptor gene (MC1R) moderated analgesic responses to pentazocine among women but not among men; however, there was no overall sex difference in pentazocine analgesia. [11] Therefore, sex differences in responses to κ-agonist–antagonists have yet to be replicated in other clinical assays or using experimental pain models.The main purpose of the current study was to investigate sex differences in pentazocine analgesia using multiple, well-validated experimental pain models. Healthy women and men underwent three experimental pain procedures (heat pain, pressure pain, and ischemic pain) before and after double-blind administration of pentazocine and saline placebo. Laboratory pain models were used because they allow greater stimulus control and the effects of pentazocine could be tested in the absence of other medications that are administered in most postoperative pain models. Based on the previous findings of Gear et al., [7] [8] we hypothesized that women would show greater analgesic responses than men.Materials and MethodsSubjectsSubjects included 41 women and 38 men recruited via posted advertisements. All participants were healthy nonsmokers and were free of clinical pain, psychiatric disturbance, substance abuse, or use of centrally acting medications. Subjects refrained from any over-the-counter medication use for at least 24 h before testing. Nineteen (46.3%) of the women were taking oral contraceptives. Based on our previous findings, [11] subjects (five women, nine men) with two variant alleles on the MC1R gene were excluded from the analyses. Subjects were paid $50 per experimental session for their participation.General Experimental ProceduresAll subjects participated in two experimental sessions, one involving administration of pentazocine and the other involving saline placebo, in randomly counterbalanced order. For women, all sessions were conducted during the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle, between days 4 and 10 after the onset of menses. Half of the women participated in the two experimental sessions within the a single menstrual cycle, separated by 2–7 days, and the other half participated across two sequential menstrual cycles, in which case the sessions were separated by approximately 28 days. To maintain consistent intervals across sex, 19 men participated in the two sessions within 1 week, and 16 men participated with the longer interval (i.e., 4 weeks).Before the experimental sessions began, all subjects provided verbal and written informed consent and completed a series of health and psychological questionnaires to ensure that all subjects were free of any medical conditions, psychological conditions, or both. The two experimental sessions were identical, except that pentazocine was administered in one session and saline was administered in the other. All sessions were conducted by two experimenters, either two women or one woman and one man. Each experimental session started with insertion of an intravenous cannula for drug administration followed by a 15-min rest period, during which blood pressure and heart rate were monitored. Next, predrug experimental pain testing was performed, including assessment of thermal pain, pressure pain, and ischemic pain (described in detail in the next section, Pain Testing Procedures). After the predrug pain testing, a 15-min rest period was observed, followed by double-blind intravenous bolus administration of either pentazocine (0.5 mg/kg) or saline, in randomized order. Fifteen minutes after drug administration, pain testing was repeated in a manner identical to the predrug testing. A timeline depicting the experimental session is presented in figure 1. Adverse effects reported by subjects, observed by experimenters, or both were also recorded. All procedures were approved by the University of Florida Health Science Center’s Institutional Review Board (Gainesville, Florida).Fig. 1. Timeline of the experimental session. The boxed text presents the components of the experimental session, and the numbers below the timeline indicate the approximate time in minutes at which experimental procedures were conducted. The lines linking thermal pain to pressure pain indicate that these two procedures were conducted in counterbalanced order.Pain Testing ProceduresThe following experimental pain procedures were conducted before and after drug administration. Pressure and thermal pain were delivered first in counterbalanced order, separated by a 5-min rest period. Ischemic pain always occurred last to reduce the possibility of carryover effects. Before each pain procedure, digitally recorded instructions were played for the subject.Pressure Pain Threshold. A handheld algometer (Pain Diagnostics and Therapeutics, Great Neck, NY) was used to assess pressure pain threshold (PPT). Mechanical pressure was applied using a 1-cm2 probe. A relatively slow application rate of 1 kg/s was used to reduce artifact associated with reaction time. Subjects were instructed to report when the pressure first became painful. PPTs were assessed at three sites: the center of the right upper trapezius (posterior to the clavicle), the right masseter (approximately midway between the ear opening and the corner of the mouth), and the right ulna (on the dorsal forearm, approximately 8 cm distal to the elbow), with the order of site presentation counterbalanced. PPTs were assessed three times at each site, and the average of the three assessments was determined and used in subsequent analyses.Thermal Pain ProceduresThreshold and Tolerance. The first thermal procedure involved assessment of heat pain threshold and tolerance. Contact heat stimuli were delivered using a computer-controlled Medoc Thermal Sensory Analyzer (TSA-2001; Ramat Yishai, Israel), which is a Peltier element-based stimulator. Temperature levels were monitored by a contactor-contained thermistor and returned to a preset baseline of 32°C by active cooling at a rate of 10°C/s. The 3 × 3-cm contact probe was applied to the right ventral forearm. In separate series of trials, warmth thresholds, heat pain thresholds, and heat pain tolerances were assessed using an ascending method of limits. From a baseline of 32°C, probe temperature increased at a rate of 0.5°C/s until the subject responded by pressing a button to indicate when he or she first felt pain and when he or she no longer felt able to tolerate the pain. This slow rise time was selected as a test of pain evoked mainly by stimulation of C-nociceptive afferents, as has been previously demonstrated. [12] [13] Four trials of heat pain threshold (HPTh) and heat pain tolerance (HPTo) were presented to each subject. The position of the thermode was altered slightly between trials (although it remained on the ventral forearm) to avoid either sensitization or response suppression of cutaneous heat nociceptors. For each measure, the average of all four trials was computed for use in subsequent analyses.Temporal Summation of Thermal Pain. After a 5-min rest period, the temporal summation procedure was conducted. This procedure involved administration of brief, repetitive, suprathreshold heat pulses to assess first and second pain and temporal summation of the latter. [14] Subjects rated thermal pain intensity of 10 repetitive heat pulses applied to the right dorsal forearm. The target temperatures were delivered for less than 1 s, with a 2.5-s interpulse interval during which the temperature of the contactor returned to a baseline of 40°C. Subjects were asked to rate the peak pain for each of the 10 heat pulses. Because subjects vary in their responses to heat pain, we examined temporal summation at two different stimulus intensities. This increased the likelihood that at least one set of stimuli would be at least moderately painful but tolerable for the majority of subjects. Therefore, two sets of target temperatures, 49° and 52°C, were used. Subjects were instructed to verbally rate the intensity of each thermal pulse using a numerical rating scale as previously described, [15] on which 0 represented no sensation, 20 represented a barely painful sensation, and 100 represented the most intense pain imaginable. Subjects were told that the procedure would be terminated when they reported a rating of 100, when 10 trials had elapsed, or when they wished to stop. Two measures from the temporal summation procedure at each temperature were used in subsequent analyses. The rating of the first trial was selected to represent a measure of first pain, and the rating of the fourth trial was selected to reflect summated second pain. These ratings were chosen for two reasons. First, using similar methods, it has been established that the most intense pain from the first pulse is that of “first pain” and that the most intense pain from the third or fourth pulse is that of “second pain.” [14] [16] This pattern results from a progressive suppression of first pain and temporal summation of second pain throughout a train of four heat pulses. Second, inspection of the mean ratings for each trial indicated that the increase in ratings was most robust through trial 4, suggesting that trial 4 best reflected temporal summation.Modified Submaximal Tourniquet ProcedureAfter the first two pain procedures, a 5-min rest period was observed, after which subjects underwent the modified submaximal tourniquet procedure. [17] [18] Next, the right arm was exsanguinated by elevating it above heart level for 30 s, after which the arm was occluded with a standard blood pressure cuff positioned proximal to the elbow and inflated to 240 mmHg using a Hokanson E20 Rapid Cuff Inflator (D.E. Hokanson, Bellevue, WA). Subjects then performed 20 handgrip exercises of 2-s duration at 4-s intervals at 50% of their maximum grip strength. Subjects were instructed to report when they first felt pain (ischemic pain threshold [IPTh]) and then to continue until the pain became intolerable (ischemic pain tolerance [IPTo]), and these time points were recorded. Every 30 s, subjects were prompted to alternately rate either the intensity or the unpleasantness of their pain using joint numerical (0–20) and verbal descriptor box scales. [19] An uninformed 15-min time limit was observed. In addition to IPTh and IPTo, two total pain scores were created, one for pain intensity and one for pain unpleasantness, by summing all ratings obtained during the procedure. To replace missing values created by subjects terminating the procedure before the time limit, the last rating provided was carried forward.DiscussionThis study examined sex differences in pain perception and pentazocine analgesia using three commonly used experimental pain models. The results indicate sex differences in baseline thermal and pressure pain responses but no differences in ischemic pain measures. This is generally consistent with previous research on sex differences in experimental pain perception, which have reported greater pain sensitivity among female subjects, with the magnitude of the difference varying across pain stimuli. [20] [21] [22] The current results suggest that 0.5 mg/kg intravenous pentazocine produced significant analgesic responses on most pain measures for both women and men; however, in contrast to our hypotheses, no sex differences in pentazocine analgesia emerged on any of the pain tasks. Similarly, no sex differences in adverse effects were observed. Although the analgesic effects of pentazocine have been demonstrated in other experimental pain models, sex differences in pentazocine analgesia were not addressed in these previous studies. Two of the four previous investigations included only male subjects, [23] [24] and the two that included both women and men did not comment on sex differences in analgesic responses. [25] [26] Interestingly, Kobal et al. [25] found sex differences in the pharmacokinetics of pentazocine, with a longer half-life and mean residence time among women than men. However, they did not report on sex differences in analgesic responses. The most compelling evidence for sex differences in pentazocine analgesia comes from two clinical studies conducted at the University of California at San Francisco using an oral surgery pain model. Gordon et al. [8] reported that women (n = 22) experienced greater analgesia from 30 mg intravenous pentazocine compared with men (n = 12), and there was a trend toward more prolonged analgesia among women. In a subsequent study using the same methodology, Gear et al. [7] reported more robust pentazocine analgesia among women (n = 10) than among men (n = 8) at 10 and 30 min after medication administration.Multiple factors may explain the discrepancy between the current findings and those of Gear et al. Perhaps the most obvious difference is the pain assay used. The pain after oral surgery differs from our experimental pain procedures in several substantial ways. First, postoperative dental pain includes a strong inflammatory component, and κ-opioid agonists produce peripheral antiinflammatory effects [27] [28] [29] [30] ; therefore, sex differences in the antiinflammatory action of pentazocine could contribute to the differences in opioid analgesia in postoperative pain models. Second, the oral surgery model involved premedication with diazepam, nitrous oxide, and a local anesthetic, and these drugs could influence pentazocine analgesia. For example, diazepam has been found to bind to κ-opioid receptors in vitro, [31] and systemically administered diazepam can attenuate both μ- and κ-opioid analgesia. [32] Moreover, benzodiazepine antagonism potentiated morphine analgesia in the oral surgery model. [33] In addition to benzodiazepines, nitrous oxide is thought to produce analgesia at least in part by activating κ-opioid receptors. [34] [35] Whether these drug interactions contribute to sex differences in pentazocine analgesia is not known. Third, in the oral surgery model, drug was delivered when the patients were experiencing at least moderate pain, whereas our subjects were pain free at the time of drug administration. This is potentially important because the effects of opioids may differ when administered in the presence of pain and inflammation versus the pain-free state.Other methodologic factors could also contribute to the discrepancy in findings. For example, we dosed by weight (0.5 mg/kg), which resulted in men receiving higher amounts of pentazocine in our study compared with the fixed dosage (30 mg) used in the University of California at San Francisco studies. Differences in the timing of postdrug assessments are unlikely to account for the conflicting results because we started our postdrug pain testing 15 min after drug administration, and it was completed within 60 min of drug administration. This timing is consistent with the period reported on in the University of California at San Francisco studies because they reported sex differences at 10 min and at 30 min in one study [7] and from 30 to 170 min in another. [8] However, Kobal et al. [25] reported that 30 mg intravenous pentazocine had a longer half-life among women than among men, although their fixed dosing resulted in women receiving a higher dose per unit body weight. Nonetheless, the longer residence time of pentazocine among women is interesting given that the only tendency toward greater analgesia for women in our data emerged for ischemic pain, the pain task that was conducted after the longest postdrug delay. In addition, previous research with morphine has shown more rapid onset of analgesia in men and longer duration of analgesia in women, [3] and nalbuphine, another κ-agonist–antagonist, produced more prolonged analgesia in women compared with men. [9] Therefore, future research exploring sex differences in opioid analgesia should evaluate sex differences in the onset and offset of drug effects.In addition, sample selection may contribute to the different study outcomes. Participants in our protocol were recruited specifically for a study involving experimental pain testing and analgesics and may represent a different population than patients presenting for third molar extraction, who are then offered participation in a clinical protocol. These additional methodologic differences notwithstanding, variability in the pain assays seems most likely to account for the differences in findings. This could be an important finding because it indicates that sex differences in analgesic responses to pentazocine and perhaps other opioids may be limited to certain types of pain. Additional research is needed to determine the conditions under which sex differences in opioid analgesia are most likely to emerge.The use of multiple pain assays in the current study yielded additional important results. First, consistent with studies of μ-opioid agonists on multiple pain tests, [16] [36] pentazocine produced larger and more reliable effects on pain predominantly associated with C-nociceptor stimulation (HPTh, HPTo, ratings of the fourth heat pulse) than pain predominantly associated with A-δ nociceptor stimulation (ratings of the first heat pulse). Also, the largest analgesic effects emerged on measures of ischemic pain, a form of tonic muscle pain that reflects a combination of A and C nociceptors from deep tissue. This form of tonic pain may better simulate many types of clinical pain, as argued by Smith et al. [37] In addition, analgesic responses showed low correlations across pain modalities (table 3). For women, analgesic responses assessed using heat pain measures were significantly correlated with analgesia determined via ischemic and pressure pain assays; however, these correlations were low in magnitude and not significantly different from the correlations in men, and no other significant correlations emerged. A similar pattern of results has been reported for baseline pain responses, in which measures based on different pain modalities are modestly correlated at best. [38] [39] This suggests that despite significant analgesic responses across all pain modalities, analgesic sensitivity determined using one type of pain is a poor predictor of analgesic response to other types of pain and that different mechanisms underlie analgesic responses for different types of pain. An important practical implication of these findings is that multiple pain assays will be required to fully characterize the analgesic effects of many drugs.Several limitations of the current study deserve mention. First, analgesic responses assessed using experimental pain models may not accurately reflect the analgesic responses that occur in the clinical setting. Experimental models offer several advantages, including control over stimulus parameters, the ability to test multiple pain modalities in the same sample, and freedom to examine analgesic responses in the absence of other medications or tissue pathology. However, the clinical relevance of analgesic responses measured against experimentally induced pain has yet to be empirically determined. As discussed in the third paragraph of the Discussion, sex differences in the analgesic effects of pentazocine may differ for inflammatory postoperative pain compared with nociceptive experimental pain. Therefore, these results do not refute the existence of sex differences in the analgesic effects of pentazocine for postoperative pain; rather, these findings suggest that sex differences in pentazocine analgesia may be specific to postoperative pain and do not represent a general phenomenon. To test this hypothesis, one would need to evaluate pentazocine analgesia against both experimental and postoperative pain in the same sample of women and men.Second, our sample consisted of healthy young adults whose responses may not generalize to other populations that may differ in health status, age, or other relevant variables. Third, we only tested one dose of pentazocine over a limited time period and were unable to determine whether sex differences are present at other doses or whether time course influences sex differences. Similarly, we did not collect any pharmacokinetic or pharmacodynamic data; therefore, we were unable to determine whether there were sex differences in duration of action or plasma concentrations of the drug. As mentioned above, our doses and timing were similar to those reported previously in studies that demonstrated sex differences in pentazocine analgesia [7] [8] ; however, these investigators have also demonstrated that sex differences in responses to other κ-agonist–antagonists are dose dependent, [10] and others have reported sex differences in the kinetics of pentazocine. [25] Additional research to determine the importance of dose as well as pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic factors to sex differences in opioid analgesia is needed.ConclusionThese limitations notwithstanding, we tested a substantially larger sample than did previous studies, which provided sufficient power to detect a sex difference of moderate magnitude. In addition, we used multiple well-validated experimental pain procedures, each of which was sensitive to the analgesic effects of pentazocine. When combined with previous research, these findings suggest that sex differences in responses to κ-agonist–antagonists may emerge only under certain conditions, and further research is needed to better characterize sex differences in responses to opioid analgesics.References1. Miaskowski C , Levine JD : Does opioid analgesia show a gender preference for females? Pain Forum 1999 ; 8 : 34–44 2. Chia YY , Chow LH , Hung CC , Liu K , Ger LP , Wang PN : Gender and pain upon movement are associated with the requirements for postoperative patient-controlled iv analgesia : A prospective survey of 2,298 Chinese patients . Can J Anaesth 2002 ; 49 : 249–55 Abstract 3. Sarton E , Olofsen E , Romberg R , den Hartigh J , Kest B , Nieuwenhuijs D , Burm A , Teppema L , Dahan A : Sex differences in morphine analgesia : An experimental study in healthy volunteers . Anesthesiology 2000 ; 93 : 1245–54 4. Dahan A , Sarton E , Teppema L , Olievier C : Sex-related differences in the influence of morphine on ventilatory control in humans . Anesthesiology 1998 ; 88 : 903–13 5. Sarton E , Teppema L , Dahan A : Sex differences in morphine induced ventilatory depression reside in the peripheral chemoreflex loop . Anesthesiology 1999 ; 90 : 1329–38 6. Zacny JP : Gender differences in opioid analgesia in human volunteers : Cold pressor and mechanical pain (CPDD abstract) . NIDA Res Monogr 2002 ; 182 : 22–3 7. Gear RW , Gordon NC , Heller PH , Paul S , Miaskowski C , Levine JD : Gender difference in analgesic response to the kappa-opioid pentazocine . Neurosci Lett 1996 ; 205 : 207–9 Abstract 8. Gordon NC , Gear RW , Heller PH , Paul S , Miaskowski C , Levine JD : Enhancement of morphine analgesia by the GABAB agonist baclofen . Neuroscience 1995 ; 69 : 345–9 Abstract 9. Gear RW , Miaskowski C , Gordon NC , Paul SM , Heller PH , Levine JD : Kappa-opioids produce significantly greater analgesia in women than in men . Nat Med 1996 ; 2 : 1248–50 Abstract 10. Gear RW , Miaskowski C , Gordon NC , Paul SM , Heller PH , Levine JD : The kappa opioid nalbuphine produces gender- and dose-dependent analgesia and antianalgesia in patients with postoperative pain . Pain 1999 ; 83 : 339–45 Abstract 11. Mogil JS , Wilson SG , Chesler EJ , Rankin AL , Nemmani KV , Lariviere WR , Groce MK , Wallace MR , Kaplan L , Staud R , Ness TJ , Glover TL , Stankova M , Mayorov A , Hruby VJ , Grisel JE , Fillingim RB : The melanocortin-1 receptor gene mediates female-specific mechanisms of analgesia in mice and humans . Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003 ; 100 : 4867–762 Abstract 12. Yeomans DC , Pirec V , Proudfit HK : Nociceptive responses to high and low rates of noxious cutaneous heating are mediated by different nociceptors in the rat : Behavioral evidence . Pain 1996 ; 68 : 133–40 Abstract 13. Yeomans DC , Proudfit HK : Nociceptive responses to high and low rates of noxious cutaneous heating are mediated by different nociceptors in the rat : Electrophysiological evidence . Pain 1996 ; 68 : 141–50 Abstract 14. Price DD , Hu JW , Dubner R , Gracely RH : Peripheral suppression of first pain and central summation of second pain evoked by noxious heat pulses . Pain 1977 ; 3 : 57–68 Abstract 15. Fillingim RB , Maixner W , Kincaid S , Silva S : Sex differences in temporal summation but not sensory-discriminative processing of thermal pain . Pain 1998 ; 75 : 121–7 Abstract 16. Price DD , Von der GA , Miller J , Rafii A , Price C : A psychophysical analysis of morphine analgesia . Pain 1985 ; 22 : 261–9 Abstract 17. Maixner W , Gracely RH , Zuniga JR , Humphrey CB , Bloodworth GR : Cardiovascular and sensory responses to forearm ischemia and dynamic hand exercise . Am J Physiol 1990 ; 259 : R1156–63 Abstract 18. Moore PA , Duncan GH , Scott DS , Gregg JM , Ghia JN : The submaximal effort tourniquet test : Its use in evaluating experimental and chronic pain . Pain 1979 ; 6 : 375–82 Abstract 19. Sternberg WF , Bailin D , Grant M , Gracely RH : Competition alters the perception of noxious stimuli in male and female athletes . Pain 1998 ; 76 : 231–8 Abstract 20. Berkley KJ : Sex differences in pain . Behav Brain Sci 1997 ; 20 : 371–80 Abstract 21. Fillingim RB , Maixner W : Gender differences in the responses to noxious stimuli . Pain Forum 1995 ; 4 : 209–21 22. Riley JL , Robinson ME , Wise EA , Myers CD , Fillingim RB : Sex differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli : A meta-analysis . Pain 1998 ; 74 : 181–7 Abstract 23. Bromm B , Ganzel R , Herrmann WM , Meier W , Scharein E : The analgesic efficacy of flupirtine in comparison to pentazocine and placebo assessed by EEG and subjective pain ratings . Postgrad Med J 1987 ; 63 ( suppl 3): 109–12 Abstract 24. Lotsch J , Ditterich W , Hummel T , Kobal G : Antinociceptive effects of the kappa-opioid receptor agonist RP 60180 compared with pentazocine in an experimental human pain model . Clin Neuropharmacol 1997 ; 20 : 224–33 Abstract 25. Kobal G , Hummel B , Nuernberg B , Brune K : Effects of pentazocine and acetylsalicylic acid on pain-rating, pain-related evoked potentials and vigilance in relationship to pharmacokinetic parameters . Agents and Actions 1990 ; 29 : 342–59 26. Stacher G , Steinringer H , Winklehner S , Mittelbach G , Schneider C : Effects of graded oral doses of meptazinol and pentazocine in comparison with placebo on experimentally induced pain in healthy humans . British J Clin Pharmacol 1983 ; 16 : 149–56 27. Binder W , Machelska H , Mousa S , Schmitt T , Riviere PJ , Junien JL , Stein C , Schafer M : Analgesic and antiinflammatory effects of two novel kappa-opioid peptides . Anesthesiology 2001 ; 94 : 1034–44 28. Binder W , Carmody J , Walker J : Effect of gender on anti-inflammatory and analgesic actions of two kappa-opioids . J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2000 ; 292 : 303–9 Abstract 29. Stein C , Machelska H , Schafer M : Peripheral analgesic and antiinflammatory effects of opioids . Z Rheumatol 2001 ; 60 : 416–24 Abstract 30. Vachon P , Moreau JP : Butorphanol decreases edema following carrageenan-induced paw inflammation in rats . Contemp Top Lab Anim Sci 2002 ; 41 : 15–7 Abstract 31. Cox RF , Collins MA : The effects of benzodiazepines on human opioid receptor binding and function . Anesth Analg 2001 ; 93 : 354–8 Full Text 32. Nemmani KV , Mogil JS : Serotonin-GABA interactions in the modulation of mu- and kappa-opioid analgesia . Neuropharmacology 2003 ; 44 : 304–10 Abstract 33. Gear RW , Miaskowski C , Heller PH , Paul SM , Gordon NC , Levine JD : Benzodiazepine mediated antagonism of opioid analgesia . Pain 1997 ; 71 : 25–9 Abstract 34. Quock RM , Best JA , Chen DC , Vaughn LK , Portoghese PS , Takemori AE : Mediation of nitrous oxide analgesia in mice by spinal and supraspinal kappa-opioid receptors . Eur J Pharmacol 1990 ; 175 : 97–100 Abstract 35. Quock RM , Mueller J : Protection by U-50,488H against beta-chlornaltrexamine antagonism of nitrous oxide antinociception in mice . Brain Res 1991 ; 549 : 162–4 Abstract 36. Price DD , Staud R , Robinson ME , Mauderli AP , Cannon R , Vierck CJ : Enhanced temporal summation of second pain and its central modulation in fibromyalgia patients . Pain 2002 ; 99 : 49–59 Abstract 37. Smith GM , Egbert LD , Markowtiz RA , Mosteller F , Beecher HK : An experimental pain method sensitive to morphine in man : The submaximum effort tourniquet technique . J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1966 ; 154 : 324–32 Citation 38. Janal MN , Glusman M , Kuhl JP , Clark WC : On the absence of correlation between responses to noxious heat, cold, electrical and ischemic stimulation . Pain 1994 ; 58 : 403–11 Abstract 39. Lautenbacher S , Rollman GB : Sex differences in responsiveness to painful and non-painful stimuli are dependent upon the stimulation method . Pain 1993 ; 53 : 255–64 Abstract Patient expectations for pain relief in the ED.Fosnocht DE - Am J Emerg Med - 01-JUL-2004; 22(4): 286-8From NIH/NLM MEDLINE Results A total of 752 patients were enrolled in the study. Complete data were available for 522 patients with pain and 144 patients without pain. The patient group without pain tended to be older and had a higher percentage of men than the patient group presenting with pain. Patient demographics are displayed in Table 1 . TABLE 1. Demographics for Patients With Pain and Patients Without Pain Total Age 18–54 yrs Age >54 yrs Mean Age (95% CI) Female Male Patients with pain 522 453 (87%) 69 (13%) 38 (±1.3) 269 (52%) 253 (48%) Patients without pain 144 106 (74%) 38 (26%) 45 (±3.1) 64 (44%) 80 (56%) Patients presenting to the ED with pain had a mean expectation for pain relief of 72% (95% CI 70 to 74). This was not significantly different from patients presenting to the ED without pain who had a mean expectation for pain relief of 74%, (95% CI 71 to 77) if they had presented with a painful injury or illness. Ninety-four of 522 (18%) patients with pain and 22 of 144 (15%) patients without pain expected complete relief of pain in the ED (VAS measurement of pain RELIEF = 100 mm). Of those patients presenting with pain, there was no difference in the mean expectation for pain relief in those with mild (VAS 1–33 mm), moderate (VAS 34–66 mm), or severe (VAS > 66 mm) pain. Mean expectations for pain relief for those patients presenting with mild, moderate, and severe pain are reported in Table 2 . Pain intensity at presentation correlated poorly with patient expectations for pain relief R = 0.150 (95% CI 0.07 to 0.23).TABLE 2. Expectations for Pain Relief VAS (95% CI) Those With Mild, Moderate, and Severe Pain * Mild pain VAS 1–33 mm, moderate pain VAS 34–66 mm and severe pain VAS >66 mm. Initial Pain Severity* Number Mean % Expectation for Pain Relief Range Mild pain 94 68 (63–73) 2 to 100 Moderate pain 157 71 (68–74) 15 to 100 Severe pain 271 75 (73–78) 1 to 100 Age and gender were not associated with differences in patient expectations for pain relief in the ED for patients with or without pain. Patient expectations for pain relief by age and gender are displayed in Table 3 . TABLE 3. Mean % Expectation for Pain Relief VAS (95% CI) by Age and Gender Age 18–54 Age >54 Females Males Patients without pain 74 (70–78) 73 (66–80) 76 (72–80) 73 (68–78) Patients with pain 73 (71–75) 70 (64–76) 73 (70–76) 72 (69–75) Discussion Relieving pain is one of the oldest and most basic principles of medicine. Despite an increasingly scientific understanding of pain and the availability of effective analgesics, inadequate pain relief is common for patients in the ED. Poor understanding of patient expectations for pain relief may contribute to poor analgesia in the ED. In addition, patient expectations for pain relief may play an important role in the development of standardized outcomes measures for pain relief in the ED setting. Various authors have suggested that patient expectations strongly influence patient’s experience of pain and satisfaction with care.[17] [18] [23] We found that ED patients have high expectations for pain relief. Patients with pain reported a mean expectation for pain relief of 72% with 18 % of patients expecting complete relief of their pain. This contrasts with studies performed in the surgical setting where postoperative patients have low expectations for pain relief.[14] [24] However, the number of patients expecting complete relief of pain is consistent with a report by Beel et al who found that in an ED population with acute fractures, 25% of patients wanted complete relief of their pain.[25] Surprisingly, ED patients’ expectations for pain relief do not appear to vary based on their initial pain intensity. Patients’ mean expectation for pain relief increased from 68% for those with mild pain to 71% for moderate pain and 75% for severe pain. These differences were not statistically significant and the corresponding correlation of pain intensity at presentation with expectations for pain relief was only slight at R = 0.150. In addition, the difference in expectations for pain relief was only 7 % (7 mm) between those with mild and those with severe pain. This difference is unlikely to be clinically significant based on the work of Todd et al and Gallagher et al who have established that 13 mm is the threshold for clinically significant differences in pain relief using a 100 mm VAS.[26] [27] ED patient expectations for pain relief may in fact be independent of an acute painful injury or illness. Our evaluation of ED patients presenting without pain revealed that they would expect 74% of their pain to be relieved if they had presented with a painful injury or illness. This is similar to those patients presenting with pain and suggests patients’ expectations for pain relief may be determined before the acute event that brings them to the ED. Factors such as previous pain experience and prior treatment of pain in an ED or other medical setting that may influence patient expectations for pain relief are deserving of further study.Age has been shown to influence patient perceptions of pain and may have an impact on patient expectations for pain relief in the ED. Studies in surgical settings show that differences exist in patient expectations for pain treatment between younger and older patients, with older patients having less pain, but requiring the same amount of analgesia as younger patients.[28] In an ED setting elderly patients were found to report lower pain intensity to a standardized painful stimulus than younger patients.[29] However, we found no significant differences in patient expectations for pain relief between younger and older patients.Gender has also been reported to impact patient’s experience of pain in various studies. Nevin has evaluated the relationship between pain and gender and concluded that women have lower pain thresholds, lower pain tolerance, and report greater pain intensity than men.[30] Beel et al studied 107 adults with acute long-bone fractures and reported that men were more likely than women to prefer no pain treatment in the ED.[25] However, gender-related expectations for pain relief were not formally quantified in any of these studies. Our study of ED patients shows no differences in patient expectations for pain relief between men and women, with both groups expecting a high degree of pain relief in the ED.This study has several limitations. First, this study represents a convenience sample of patients. This may have led to selection bias in the enrollment of patients and may limit the extent to which the results may be generalized. The total number of patients presenting to the ED with and without pain during the study period is unknown. However, an attempt was made to enroll all patients meeting the study criteria when research associates were available for data collection. The rate of refusal for patients approached to be enrolled in the study was less than 1%. Second, demographics between patients with pain and patients without pain are similar, but not identical. This may have led to inaccurate conclusions about the similarities or differences in expectations for pain relief between these subgroups. Third, we made no attempt to limit inclusion by acuity of pain or type of injury or illness. Pain duration and chief complaint may impact patient expectations for pain relief in the ED. Although our results are likely to be representative of an overall ED population, they may not accurately reflect expectations for pain relief of patients with specific injuries or illnesses. Finally, this study was conducted in a single university ED and the results may not be applicable to other practice settings.In summary, patients expect a high degree of pain relief in the ED with a mean expectation of 72 % relief of their pain. Eighteen percent of patients expect complete relief of pain. Patient expectations for pain relief are poorly correlated with initial pain intensity. ED patients without pain reported similar expectations for pain relief if they had presented with pain, suggesting that patient expectations for pain relief may be independent of the presence of an acute injury or illness. Patient age and gender do not appear to influence patient expectations for pain relief in the ED.References1. JCAHO. Pain Management Standards for 2001 Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations; 2001. p. 1.2.7-8. 2. Goodacre SW, Roden RK. A protocol to improve analgesia use in the accident and emergency department. J Accid Emerg Med 1996;13:177-9. Abstract 3. Ducharme J. Emergency pain managementA Canadian Association of Emergency Physicians (CAEP) consensus document. J Emerg Med 1994;12:855-66. Abstract 4. Ducharme J, Barber C. A prospective blinded study on emergency pain assessment and therapy. J Emerg Med 1995;13:571-5. Abstract 5. Johnston CC, Gagnon AJ, Fullerton L, et al. One-week survey of pain intensity on admission to and discharge from the emergency departmenta pilot study. J Emerg Med 1998;16:377-82. Abstract 6. Jones JB. Assessment of pain management skills in emergency medicine residentsthe role of a pain education program. J Emerg Med 1999;17:349-54. Abstract 7. Lewis LM, Lasater LC, Brooks CB. Are emergency physicians too stingy with analgesics? South Med J 1994;87:7-9. Abstract 8. Ngai B, Ducharme J. Documented use of analgesics in the emergency department and upon release of patients with extremity fractures [letter]. Acad Emerg Med 1997;4:1176-8. Citation 9. Petrack EM, Christopher NC, Kriwinsky J. Pain management in the emergency departmentpatterns of analgesic utilization. Pediatrics 1997;99:711-4. Abstract 10. Selbst SM, Clark M. Analgesic use in the emergency department. Ann Emerg Med 1990;19:1010-3. Abstract 11. Wilson JE, Pendleton JM. Oligoanalgesia in the emergency department. Am J Emerg Med 1989;7:620-3. Abstract 12. Singer AJ, Richman PB, Kowalska A, et al. Comparison of patient and practitioner assessments of pain from commonly performed emergency department procedures. Ann Emerg Med 1999;33:652-8. Full Text 13. Fosnocht DE, Swanson ER, Bossart P. Patient expectations for pain medication delivery. Am J Emerg Med 2001;19:399-402. Full Text 14. Kuhn S, Cooke K, Collins M, et al. Perceptions of pain relief after surgery. BMJ 1990;300:1687-90. Abstract 15. Kelly A. Patient satisfaction with pain management does not correlate with initial or discharge VAS pain score, verbal pain rating at discharge, or change in VAS score in the emergency department. J Emerg Med 2000;19:113-6. Abstract 16. Ward SE, Gordon D. Application of the American Pain Society quality assurance standards. Pain 1994;56:299-306. Abstract 17. Afilalo M, Tselios C. Pain relief versus patient satisfaction. Ann Emerg Med 1996;27:436-8. Full Text 18. Carragee EJ, Vittom D, Truong TP, et al. Pain control and cultural norms and expectations after closed femoral shaft fractures. Am J Orthop 1999;28:97-102. Abstract 19. Huskisson E. Visual Analog scales. In: MelzackR, editors. Pain Measurement and Assessment New York: Raven Press; 1983. p. 33-7. 20. Menegazzi J. Measuring pain at baseline and over time. Ann Emerg Med 1996;27:433-5. Full Text 21. Wallenstein S. Scaling clinical pain and pain relief. In: BrommB, editors. Pain Measurement in ManNeurophysiological Correlates of Pain New York: Elsevier Science Publishers; 1984. p. 389-94. 22. Chapman C, Richard SKL. Measurement of pain. In: LoeserJD, ButlerSH, ChapmanC, editors. Bonica’s Management of Pain Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2001. p. 310-28. 23. McGrath PA. Psychological aspects of pain perception. Arch Oral Biol 1994;39(suppl):55S-62S. Abstract 24. Lavies N, Hart L, Rounsefell B, et al. Identification of patient, medical and nursing staff attitudes to postoperative opioid analgesiastage 1 of a longitudinal study of postoperative analgesia. Pain 1992;48:313-9. Abstract 25. Beel TL, Mitchiner JC, Frederiksen SM, et al. Patient preferences regarding pain medication in the ED. Am J Emerg Med 2000;18:376-80. Full Text 26. Gallagher EJ, Liebman M, Bijur PE. Prospective validation of clinically important changes in pain severity measured on a visual analog scale. Ann Emerg Med 2001;38:633-8. Full Text 27. Todd KH, Funk KG, Funk JP, et al. Clinical significance of reported changes in pain severity. Ann Emerg Med 1996;27:485-9. Full Text 28. Perry F, Parker RK, White PF, et al. Role of psychological factors in postoperative pain control and recovery with patient-controlled analgesia. Clin J Pain 1994;10:57-63. discussion 82–5 Abstract 29. Li SF, Greenwald PW, Gennis P, et al. Effect of age on acute pain perception of a standardized stimulus in the emergency department. Ann Emerg Med 2001;38:644-7. Full Text 30. Nevin K. Influence of sex on pain assessment and management. Ann Emerg Med 1996;27:424-6. Full Text About MD Consult Contact Us Terms and Conditions Privacy Policy Registered User Agreement Copyright © 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. www.mdconsult.com Bookmark URL: /das/journal/view/42181205-2/N/14897669?ja=428436&PAGE=1.html&ANCHOR=top&source=MI